Sentence

Sentence (linguistics)

A sentence is a grammatical unit consisting of one or more words that are grammatically linked. A sentence can include words grouped meaningfully to express a statement, question, exclamation, request, command or suggestion.

 

A sentence can also be defined in orthographic terms alone, i.e., as anything which is contained between a capital letter and a full stop.[2] For instance, the opening of Charles Dickens' novel Bleak House begins with the following three sentences:

 

London. Michaelmas term lately over, and the Lord Chancellor sitting in Lincoln's Inn Hall.

 

Implacable November weather.

 

The first sentence involves one word, a proper noun. The second sentence has only a non-finite verb. The third is a single nominal group. Only an orthographic definition encompasses this variation.

 

As with all language expressions, sentences might contain function and content words and contain properties distinct to natural language, such as characteristic intonation and timing patterns.

 

Sentences are generally characterized in most languages by the presence of a finite verb, e.g.

 

"The quick brown fox jumps over the lazy dog".

 

 

 

Components of a sentence

 

Clauses

 

A clause typically contains at least a subject noun phrase and a finite verb. While the subject is usually a noun phrase, other kinds of phrases (such as gerund phrases) work as well, and some languages allow subjects to be omitted. There are two types of clauses: independent and subordinate (dependent). An independent clause demonstrates a complete thought; it is a complete sentence: for example, I am sad. A subordinate clause is not a complete sentence: for example, because I have no friends. See also copula for the consequences of the verb to be on the theory of sentence structure.

 

A simple complete sentence consists of a single clause. Other complete sentences consist of two or more clauses (see below).


Classification

 

By structure

 

One traditional scheme for classifying English sentences is by the number and types of finite clauses:

 

A simple sentence consists of a single independent clause with no dependent clauses.

 

A compound sentence consists of multiple independent clauses with no dependent clauses. These clauses are joined together using conjunctions, punctuation, or both.

 

A complex sentence consists of one independent clause and at least one dependent clause.

 

A complex-compound sentence (or compound-complex sentence) consists of multiple independent clauses, at least one of which has at least one dependent clause.

 

By purpose

 

Sentences can also be classified based on their purpose:

 

A declarative sentence or declaration, the most common type, commonly makes a statement: "I have to go to work."

 

An interrogative sentence or question is commonly used to request information — "Do I have to go to work?" — but sometimes not; see rhetorical question.

 

An exclamatory sentence or exclamation is generally a more emphatic form of statement expressing emotion: "I have to go to work!"

 

An imperative sentence or command tells someone to do something (and if done strongly may be considered both imperative and exclamatory): "Go to work." or "Go to work!"

 

Major and minor sentences

 

A major sentence is a regular sentence; it has a subject and a predicate. For example: "I have a ball." In this sentence one can change the persons: "We have a ball." However, a minor sentence is an irregular type of sentence. It does not contain a finite verb. For example, "Mary!" "Yes." "Coffee." etc. Other examples of minor sentences are headings (e.g. the heading of this entry), stereotyped expressions ("Hello!"), emotional expressions ("Wow!"), proverbs, etc. This can also include nominal sentences like "The more, the merrier". These do not contain verbs in order to intensify the meaning around the nouns and are normally found in poetry and catchphrases.[3]

 

Sentences that comprise a single word are called word sentences, and the words themselves sentence words


Sentence length

 

After a slump of interest, sentence length came to be studied in the 1980s, mostly "with respect to other syntactic phenomena"

 

By some definitions, the average size length of a sentence is given by "no. of words / no. of sentences".[6] The textbook Mathematical linguistics, written by András Kornai, suggests that in "journalistic prose the median sentence length is above 15 words". The average length of a sentence generally serves as a measure of sentence difficulty or complexity.[8] The general trend is that as the average sentence length increases, the complexity of the sentences also increases

 

In some circumstances "sentence length" is expressed by the number of clauses, while the "clause length" is expressed by the number of phones.

 

A test done by Erik Schils and Pieter de Haan (by sampling five texts) showed that any two adjacent sentences are more likely to have similar lengths, and almost certainly have similar length when from a text in the fiction genre. This countered the theory that "authors may aim at an alternation of long and short sentence".Sentence length, as well as word difficulty, are both factors in the readability of a sentence. However, other factors, such as the presence of conjunctions, have been said to "facilitate comprehension considerably".[

 

Simple sentence

 

A simple sentence is a sentence structure that contains one independent clause and no dependent clauses.

 

Examples

 

I am running.

 

This simple sentence has one independent clause which contains one subject, I, and one predicate, running.

 

The singer bowed.

 

This simple sentence has one independent clause which contains one subject, singer, and one predicate, bowed.

 

The babies cried.

 

This simple sentence has one independent clause which contains one subject, baby, and one predicate, cried.

 

The girl ran into her bedroom.


 

This simple sentence has one independent clause which contains one subject, girl, and one predicate, ran into her bedroom. This example is distinct from the previous three in that its verb phrase consists of more than one word.

 

In the backyard, the dog barked and howled at the cat.

 

This simple sentence has one independent clause which contains one subject, dog, and one predicate, barked and howled at the cat. This predicate has two verbs, known as a compound predicate: barked and howled. This compound verb should not be confused with a compound sentence. In the backyard and at the cat are prepositional phrases.

 

Compound sentence

 

A compound sentence is composed of at least two independent clauses. It does not require a dependent clause. The clauses are joined by a coordinating conjunction (with or without a comma), a correlative conjunction (with or without a comma), a semicolon that functions as a conjunction, a colon instead of a semicolon between two sentences when the second sentence explains or illustrates the first sentence and no coordinating conjunction is being used to connect the sentences, or a conjunctive adverb preceded by a semicolon. A conjunction can be used to make a compound sentence. Conjunctions are words such as for, and, nor, but, or, yet, so (the first letters of which spell "fanboys"). The use of a comma to separate two independent clauses without the addition of an appropriate conjunction is called a comma splice and is generally considered an error (when used in the English language).

 

 

 

Complex sentence

 

In grammar, a complex sentence is a sentence with one independent clause and at least one dependent clause. A complex sentence is often used to make clear which ideas are most important, and which ideas are subordinate

 

Examples

 

"I enjoyed the apple pie that you bought for me." Here, "I enjoyed the apple pie" is an independent clause and "that you bought for me" is a dependent clause. The independent clause could stand alone as a simple sentence without a dependent clause.

 

"I ate breakfast before I went to work." This has "I ate breakfast" as an independent clause, and "before I went to work" as a dependent clause.

 

Examples of sentences that have more than one clause but are not complex sentences include the following:


"I was scared, but I didn't run away." Both of these clauses are independent in this compound sentence.

 

"The dog that you gave me barked at me, and it bit my hand." Here a compound-complex sentence has two independent clauses ("The dog barked at me" and "It bit my hand") and one dependent clause ["that you gave me"].

 

 

 

Complex-compound sentence

 

A    complex-compound sentence or compound-complex sentence is a sentence with several independent clauses and one or more dependent clauses (also known as subordinating conjunction).

 

Examples

 

The dog lived in the garden, but the cat, which was smarter, lived inside the house.

 

Independent clauses:

 

The dog lived in the garden.

 

The cat lived inside the house.

 

Dependent clause:

 

which was smarter

 

 

 

Subject

 

The subject (abbreviated SUB or SU) is, according to a tradition that can be traced back to Aristotle (and that is associated with phrase structure grammars), one of the two main constituents of a clause, the other constituent being the predicate, whereby the predicate says something about the subject. According to a tradition associated with predicate logic and dependency grammars, the subject is the most prominent overt argument of the predicate. By this position all languages with arguments have subjects, though there is no way to define this consistently for all languages.[3] From a functional perspective, a subject is a phrase that conflates nominative case with the topic. Many languages (such as those withergative or Austronesian alignment) do not do this, and so do not have subjects.

 

All of these positions see the subject in English determining person and numberagreement on the finite verb, as exemplified by the difference in verb forms between he eats and they eat. The stereotypical subject immediately precedes the finite verb in declarative sentences in English and


 

represents an agent or a theme. The subject is often a multi-word constituent and should be distinguished fromparts of speech, which, roughly, classify words within constituents.

 

Forms of the subject

 

The subject is a constituent that can be realized in numerous forms in English and other languages, many of which are listed in the following table:

 

oun (phrase) or pronoun The large car stopped outside our house. A gerund (phrase)

 

His constant hammering was annoying.

 

A to-infinitive (phrase) To read is easier than to write.

 

A full that-clause That he had traveled the world was known to everyone.

 

A free relative clause Whatever he did was always of interest.

 

A direct quotation I love you is often heard these days.

 

Zero (but implied) subject Take out the trash!

 

An expletive

 

It is raining.

 

A cataphoric it

 

It was known by everyone that he had traveled the world.

 

Three criteria for identifying subjects in English

 

Three criteria for identifying subjects in English and other languages are listed next:

 

1.   Subject-verb agreement: The subject agrees with the finite verb in person and number, e.g. I am vs. *I is.

 

2.   Position occupied: The subject typically immediately precedes the finite verb in declarative clauses in English, e.g. Tom laughs.

 

3.   Semantic role: A typical subject in the active voice is an agent or theme, i.e. it performs the action expressed by the verb or when it is a theme, it receives a property assigned to it by the predicate.

 

Of these three criteria, the first one (agreement) is the most reliable. The subject in English and many other languages agrees with the finite verb in person and number (and sometimes in gender


 

as well). The second and third criterion are merely strong tendencies that can be flouted in certain constructions, e.g.

 

a. Tom is studying chemistry. - The three criteria agree identifying Tom as the subject.

 

b. Is Tom studying chemistry? - The 1st and the 3rd criteria identify Tom as the subject.

 

c. Chemistry is being studied (by Tom). - The 1st and the 2nd criteria identify Chemistry as the subject. In the first sentence, all three criteria combine to identify Tom as the subject. In the second sentence, which involves the subject-auxiliary inversion of a yes/no-question, the subject immediately follows the finite verb (instead of immediately preceding it), which means the second criterion is flouted. And in the third sentence expressed in the passive voice, the 1st and the 2nd criterion combine to identify chemistry as the subject, whereas the third criterion suggests that by Tom should be the subject because Tom is an agent.

 

Two further criteria for identifying subjects

 

Two further critiera for identifying subjects are helpful in many other languages (other than

 

English):

 

4.   Morphological case: In languages that have case systems, the subject is marked by a specific case, often the nominative.

 

5.  Omission: Many languages systematically omit a subject that is known in discourse.

 

The fourth criterion is not very applicable to English because English largely lacks morphological case, the exception being the subject and object forms of pronouns, I/me, he/him, she/her, they/them. The fifth criterion is helpful in languages that typically drop pronominal subjects, such as Spanish, Portuguese, Italian, Latin, Greek, Japanese, and Mandarin. Though most of these languages are rich in verb forms for determining the person and number of the subject, Japanese and Mandarin have no such forms at all. This dropping pattern does not automatically make a language a pro-drop language. In other languages, like English and French, most clauses should have a subject, which should be either a noun (phrase), a pronoun, or a clause. This is also true when the clause has no element to be represented by it. This is why verbs like rain must have a subject such as it, even if nothing is actually being represented by it. In this case, it is an expletive and a dummy pronoun. In imperative clauses, most languages elide the subject, even in English which typically requires a subject to be present, e.g.

 

Give it to me.

 

Dā mihi istud. (Latin)

 

Me dá isso. (Brazilian Portuguese)

 

Dá-me isso. (Portuguese in Brazil)


Dámelo. (Spanish)

 

Dammelo. (Italian)

 

Difficult cases

 

There are certain constructions that challenge the criteria just introduced for identifying subjects. The following subsections briefly illustrate three such cases in English: 1) existential there-constructions, 2) inverse copular constructions, and 3) locative inversionconstructions.

 

Existential there-constructions

 

Existential there-constructions allow for varying interpretations about what should count as the subject, e.g.

 

A.    There's problems.

 

B.    There are problems.

 

In sentence a, the first criterion (agreement) and the second criterion (position occupied) suggest that there is the subject, whereas the third criterion (semantic role) suggests rather that problems is the subject. In sentence b, in contrast, agreement and semantic role suggest that problems is the subject, whereas position occupied suggests that there is the subject. In such cases then, one can take the first criterion as the most telling; the subject should agree with the finite verb.

 

Inverse copular constructions

 

Another difficult case for identifying the subject is the so-called inverse copular construction, e.g.

 

A.    The boys are a chaotic force around here.

 

B.    A chaotic force around here is the boys. - Inverse copular construction

 

The criteria combine to identify the boys as the subject in sentence a. But if that is the case, then one might argue that the boys is also the subject in the similar sentence b, even though two of the criteria (agreement and position occupied) suggest that a chaotic force around here is the subject. When confronted with such data, one has to make a decision that is less than fully arbitrary. If one assumes again that criterion one (agreement) is the most reliable, one can usually identify a subject.

 

Locative inversion constructions

 

Yet another type of construction that challenges the subject concept is locative inversion, e.g.

 

A.  Spiders have been breeding under the bed.


B.    Under the bed have been breeding spiders. - Locative inversion

 

C.    *Where have been breeding spiders? - Failed attempt to question the location

 

D.    Where have spiders been breeding? - Successful attempt to question the location

 

The criteria easily identify spiders as the subject in sentence a. In sentence b, however, the position occupied suggests that under the bed should be construed as the subject, whereas agreement and semantic role continue to identify spiders as the subject. This is so despite the fact that spiders in sentence b appears after the string of verbs in the canonical position of an object. The fact that sentence c is bad but sentence d good reveals that something unusual is indeed afoot, since the attempt to question the location fails if the subject does not immediately follow the finite verb. This further observation speaks against taking spiders as the subject in sentence b. But if spiders is not the subject, then the sentence must lack a subject entirely, which is not supposed to be possible in English.

 

Subject-less clauses

 

The existence of subject-less clauses can be construed as particularly problematic for theories of sentence structure that build on the binary subject-predicate division. A simple sentence is defined as the combination of a subject and a predicate, but if no subject is present, how can one have a sentence? Subject-less clauses are absent from English for the most part, but they are not unusual in related languages. In German, for instance, impersonal passive clauses can lack a recognizable subject, e.g.

 

Gestern wurde nur geschlafen.

 

Yesterday was only slept 'Everybody slept yesterday.'

 

The word gestern 'yesterday' is generally construed as an adverb, which means it cannot be taken as the subject in this sentence. Certain verbs in German also require a dative or accusative object instead of a nominative subject, e.g.

 

Mir graut davor.

 

Me-DAT is uneasy about it 'I am uneasy about it.'

 

Since subjects are typically marked by the nominative case in German (the fourth criterion above), one can argue that this sentence lacks a subject, for the relevant verb argument appears in the dative case, not in the nominative.


 

 

Predicate

 

There are two competing notions of the predicate in theories of grammar The first concerns traditional grammar, which tends to view a predicate as one of two main parts of a sentence, the other part being the subject; the purpose of the predicate is to modify the subject. The second derives from work in predicate calculus (predicate logic, first order logic) and is prominent in modern theories of syntax and grammar. In this approach, the predicate of a sentence corresponds mainly to the main verb and any auxiliaries that accompany the main verb, whereas the arguments of that predicate (e.g. the subject and object noun phrases) are outside the predicate. The competition between these two concepts has generated confusion concerning the use of the term predicate in theories of grammar. This article considers both of these notions.

 

Predicates in traditional grammar

 

The predicate in traditional grammar is inspired by propositional logic of antiquity (as opposed to the more modern predicate logic). A predicate is seen as a property that a subject has or is characterized by. A predicate is therefore an expression that can be true ofsomething.[3] Thus, the expression "is moving" is true of those things that are moving. This classical understanding of predicates was adopted more or less directly into Latin and Greek grammars and from there it made its way into English grammars, where it is applied directly to the analysis of sentence structure. It is also the understanding of predicates in English-language dictionaries. The predicate is one of the two main parts of a sentence (the other being the subject, which the predicate modifies).[4] The predicate must contain a verb, and the verb requires, permits, or precludes other sentence elements to complete the predicate. These elements are:objects (direct, indirect, prepositional), predicatives, and adjuncts:

 

She dances. - verb-only predicate

 

Ben reads the book. - verb + direct object predicate

 

Ben's mother, Felicity, gave me a present. - verb + indirect object + direct object predicate

 

She listened to the radio. - verb + prepositional object predicate

 

They elected him president. - verb + object + predicative noun predicate

 

She met him in the park. - verb + object + adjunct predicate

 

She is in the park. - verb + predicative prepositional phrase predicate

 

The predicate provides information about the subject, such as what the subject is, what the subject is doing, or what the subject is like. The relation between a subject and its predicate is sometimes called a nexus. A predicative nominal is a noun phrase that functions as the main


 

predicate of a sentence, such as George III is the king of England, the king of England being the predicative nominal. The subject and predicative nominal must be connected by a linking verb, also called a copula. A predicative adjective is anadjective that functions as a predicate, such as Ivano is attractive, attractive being the predicative adjective. The subject and predicative adjective must also be connected by a copula.

 

This traditional understanding of predicates has a concrete reflex in all phrase structure theories of syntax. These theories divide the generic declarative sentence (S) into a noun phrase (NP) and verb phrase (VP), e.g

 

Predicates in modern theories of syntax and grammar

 

Most modern theories of syntax and grammar take their inspiration for the theory of predicates from predicate calculus as associated with Gottlob Frege. This understanding sees predicates as relations or functions over arguments. The predicate serves either to assign a property to a single argument or to relate two or more arguments to each other. Sentences consist of predicates and their arguments (and adjuncts) and are thus predicate-argument structures, whereby a given predicate is seen as linking its arguments into a greater structure. This understanding of predicates sometimes renders a predicate and its arguments in the following manner:

 

Bob laughed. → laughed (Bob) or, laughed = ƒ(Bob)

 

Sam helped you. → helped (Sam, you)

 

Jim gave Jill his dog. → gave (Jim, Jill, his dog)

 

Predicates are placed on the left outside of brackets, whereas the predicate's arguments are placed inside the brackets One acknowledges the valency of predicates, whereby a given predicate can be avalent (not shown), monovalent (laughed in the first sentence), divalent (helped in the second sentence), or trivalent (gave in the third sentence). These types of representations are analogous to formal semantic analyses, where one is concerned with the proper account of scope facts of quantifiers and logical operators. Concerning basic sentence structure however, these representations suggest above all that verbs are predicates and the noun phrases that they appear with are their arguments. On this understanding of the sentence, the binary division of the clause into a subject NP and a predicate VP is hardly possible. Instead, the verb is the predicate, and the noun phrases are its arguments.

 

Other function words - e.g. auxiliary verbs, certain prepositions, phrasal particles, etc. - are viewed as part of the predicate.The matrix predicates are in bold in the following examples:

 

Bill will have laughed.

 

Will Bill have laughed?

 

That is funny.


Has that been funny?

 

They had been satisfied.

 

Had they been satisfied, ...

 

The butter is in the drawer.

 

Fred took a picture of Sue.

 

Susan is pulling your leg.

 

Who did Jim give his dog to?

 

You should give it up.

 

Note that not just verbs can be part of the matrix predicate, but also adjectives, nouns, prepositions, etc. The understanding of predicates suggested by these examples sees the main predicate of a clause consisting of at least one verb and a variety of other possible words. The words of the predicate need not form a string nor a constituent, but rather they can be interrupted by their arguments (and/or adjuncts). The approach to predicates illustrated with these sentences is widespread in Europe, particularly in Germany, where the understanding predicates from traditional grammar discussed above seems to hardly exist (for those who know German, see the Wikipedia article in German on the predicate).

 

This modern understanding of predicates is compatible with the dependency grammar approach to sentence structure, which places the finite verb as the root of all structure

 

Predicators

 

Some theories of grammar seek to avoid the confusion generated by the competition between the two predicate notions by acknowledging predicators.[13] The term predicate is employed in the traditional sense of the binary division of the clause, whereas the term predicator is used to denote the more modern understanding of matrix predicates. On this approach, the periphrastic verb catenae briefly illustrated in the previous section are predicators

 

The predicators are in blue. These verb catenae generally contain a main verb and potentially one or more auxiliary verbs. The auxiliary verbs help express functional meaning of aspect and voice. Since the auxiliary verbs contribute functional information only, they do not qualify as separate predicators, but rather each time they form the matrix predicator with the main verb.

 

Carlson classes

 

The seminal work of Greg Carlson distinguishes between types of predicates. Based on Carlson's work, predicates have been divided into the following sub-classes, which roughly pertain to how a predicate relates to its subject.


Stage-level predicates

 

A stage-level predicate is true of a temporal stage of its subject. For example, if John is "hungry", then he typically will eat some food, which lasts a certain amount of time, and not his entire lifespan. Stage-level predicates can occur in a wide range of grammatical constructions and are probably the most versatile kind of predicate.

 

Individual-level predicates

 

An individual-level predicate is true throughout the existence of an individual. For example, if John is "smart", this is a property that he has, regardless of which particular point in time we consider. Individual-level predicates are more restricted than stage-level ones. Individual-level predicates cannot occur in presentational "there" sentences (a star in front of a sentence indicates that it is odd or ill-formed):

 

There are police available. - available is stage-level predicate

 

*There are firemen altruistic. - altruistic is an individual-level predicate

 

Stage-level predicates allow modification by manner adverbs and other adverbial modifiers.

 

Individual-level predicates do not, e.g.

 

Tyrone spoke French loudly in the corridor. - speak French can be interpreted as a stage-level predicate

 

*Tyrone knew French silently in the corridor. - know French cannot be interpreted as a stage-level predicate

 

When an individual-level predicate occurs in past tense, it gives rise to what is called a lifetime effect: The subject must be assumed to be dead or otherwise out of existence.

 

John was available. - Stage-level predicate does NOT evoke the lifetime effect.

 

John was altruistic. - Individual-level predicate does evoke the lifetime effect.

 

Kind-level predicates

 

A kind-level predicate is true of a kind of thing, but cannot be applied to individual members of the kind. An example of this is the predicate are widespread. One cannot meaningfully say of a particular individual John that he is widespread. One may only say this of kinds, as in

 

Humans are widespread.

 

Certain types of noun phrases cannot be the subject of a kind-level predicate. We have just seen that a proper name cannot be.Singular indefinite noun phrases are also banned from this environment:


*A cat is widespread. - Compare: Nightmares are widespread.

 

Collective vs. distributive predicates

 

Predicates may also be collective or distributive. Collective predicates require their subjects to be somehow plural, while distributive ones do not. An example of a collective predicate is "formed a line". This predicate can only stand in a nexus with a plural subject:

 

The students formed a line. - Collective predicate appears with plural subject.

 

*The student formed a line. - Collective predicate cannot appear with singular subject.

 

Other examples of collective predicates include meet in the woods, surround the house, gather in the hallway and carry the piano together. Note that the last one (carry the piano together) can be made non-collective by removing the word together. Quantifiers differ with respect to whether or not they can be the subject of a collective predicate. For example, quantifiers formed with all the can, while ones formed with every or each cannot.

 

All the students formed a line. - Collective predicate possible with all the.

 

All the students gathered in the hallway. - Collective predicate possible with all the.

 

All the students carried a piano together. - Collective predicate possible with all the.

 

*Every student formed a line. - Collective predicate IMpossible with every.

 

*Each student gathered in the hallway. - Collective predicate IMpossible with each.